搜档网
当前位置:搜档网 › 投资学10版习题答案12

投资学10版习题答案12

投资学10版习题答案12
投资学10版习题答案12

CHAPTER 12: BEHAVIORAL FINANCE

AND TECHNICAL ANALYSIS

PROBLEM SETS

1. Technical analysis can generally be viewed as a search for trends

or patterns in market prices. Technical analysts tend to view

these trends as momentum, or gradual adjustments to ‘correct’

prices, or, alternatively, reversals of trends. A number of the

behavioral biases discussed in the chapter might contribute to

such trends and patterns. For example, a conservatism bias might

contribute to a trend in prices as investors gradually take new

information into account, resulting in gradual adjustment of

prices towards their fundamental values. Another example derives

from the concept of representativeness, which leads investors to

inappropriately conclude, on the basis of a small sample of data, that a pattern has been established that will continue well into

the future. When investors subsequently become aware of the fact

that prices have overreacted, corrections reverse the initial

erroneous trend.

2. Even if many investors exhibit behavioral biases, security prices

might still be set efficiently if the actions of arbitrageurs move prices to their intrinsic values. Arbitrageurs who observe

mispricing in the securities markets would buy underpriced

securities (or possibly sell short overpriced securities) in order to profit from the anticipated subsequent changes as prices move to their intrinsic values. Consequently, securities prices would still exhibit the characteristics of an efficient market.

3. One of the major factors limiting the ability of rational investors

to take advantage of any ‘pricing errors’ that result from the

actions of behavioral investors is the fact that a mispricing can get worse over time. An example of this fundamental risk is the

apparent ongoing overpricing of the NASDAQ index in the late 1990s.

Related factors are the inherent costs and limits related to short selling, which restrict the extent to which arbitrage can force

overpriced securities (or indexes) to move towards their fair values. Rational investors must also be aware of the risk that an apparent mispricing is, in fact, a consequence of model risk; that is, the perceived mispricing may not be real because the investor has used a faulty model to value the security.

4. There are two reasons why behavioral biases might not affect

equilibrium asset prices: first, behavioral biases might contribute to the success of technical trading rules as prices gradually

adjust towards their intrinsic values, and second, the actions of arbitrageurs might move security prices towards their intrinsic

values. It might be important for investors to be aware of these

biases because either of these scenarios might create the potential for excess profits even if behavioral biases do not affect

equilibrium prices.

In addition, an investor should be aware of his personal behavioral biases, even if those biases do not affect equilibrium prices, to help avoid some of these information processing errors (e.g.

overconfidence or representativeness).

5. Efficient market advocates believe that publicly available

information (and, for advocates of strong-form efficiency, even

insider information) is, at any point in time, reflected in

securities prices, and that price adjustments to new information

occur very quickly. Consequently, prices are at fair levels so that active management is very unlikely to improve performance above

that of a broadly diversified index portfolio. In contrast,

advocates of behavioral finance identify a number of investor

errors in information processing and decision making that could

result in mispricing of securities. However, the behavioral finance literature generally does not provide guidance as to how these

investor errors can be exploited to generate excess profits.

Therefore, in the absence of any profitable alternatives, even if securities markets are not efficient, the optimal strategy might

still be a passive indexing strategy.

6. a. Davis uses loss aversion as the basis for her decision making.

She holds on to stocks that are down from the purchase price in the hopes that they will recover. She is reluctant to accept a loss.

7. a. Shrum refuses to follow a stock after she sells it because she

does not want to experience the regret of seeing it rise. The

behavioral characteristic used for the basis for her decision

making is the fear of regret.

8. a. Investors attempt to avoid regret by holding on to losers hoping

the stocks will rebound. If the stock rebounds to its original

purchase price, the stock can be sold with no regret. Investors

also may try to avoid regret by distancing themselves from their

decisions by hiring a full-service broker.

9. a. iv

b. iii

c. v

d. i

e. ii

10. Underlying risks still exist even during a mispricing event. The

market mispricing could get worse before it gets better. Other

adverse effects could occur before the price corrects itself (e.g., loss of clients with no understanding or appetite for mispricing opportunities).

11. Data mining is the process by which patterns are pulled from data.

Technical analysts must be careful not to engage in data mining as great is the human capacity to discern patterns where no patterns exist. Technical analysts must avoid mining data to support a

theory rather than using data to test a theory.

12. Even if prices follow a random walk, the existence of irrational

investors combined with the limits to arbitrage by arbitrageurs may allow persistent mispricings to be present. This implies that

capital will not be allocated efficiently —capital does not

immediately flow from relatively unproductive firms to relatively productive firms.

13. Trin =advancing Num ber /advancing V olum e declining Num ber /declining V olum e =

This trin ratio, which is above 1.0, would be taken as a bearish signal.

14. Breadth:

Breadth is negative —bearish signal (no one would actually use a one-

day measure).

15. This exercise is left to the student; answers will vary, but

successful students should be able to identify time periods when upward or downward trends are obvious. This exercise also shows the benefit of hindsight, which investors do not possess when

making current decisions.

16. The confidence index increases from (5%/6%) = 0.833 to (6%/7%)

= 0.857.

This indicates slightly higher confidence which would be

interpreted by technicians as a bullish signal. But the real

reason for the increase in the index is the expectation of

higher inflation, not higher confidence about the economy.

17. At the beginning of the period, the price of Computers, Inc.

divided by the industry index was 0.39; by the end of the period, the ratio had increased to 0.50. As the ratio increased over the period, it appears that Computers, Inc. outperformed other firms in its industry. The overall trend, therefore, indicates relative

strength, although some fluctuation existed during the period, with the ratio falling to a low point of 0.33 on day 19.

18. Five day moving averages:

Days 1 – 5: (19.63 + 20 + 20.5 + 22 + 21.13) / 5 = 20.65

Days 2 – 6 = 21.13

Days 3 – 7 = 21.50

Days 4 – 8 = 21.90

Days 5 – 9 = 22.13

Days 6 – 10 = 22.68

Days 7 – 11 = 23.18

Days 8 – 12 = 23.45 Sell signal (day 12 price < moving

average)

Days 9 – 13 = 23.38

Days 10 – 14 = 23.15

Advances

Declines

Net Advances 1,455 1,553 -98

Days 11 – 15 = 22.50

Days 12 – 16 = 21.65

Days 13 – 17 = 20.95

Days 14 – 18 = 20.28

Days 15 – 19 = 19.38

Days 16 – 20 = 19.05

Days 17 – 21 = 18.93Buy signal (day 21 price > moving

average)

Days 18 – 22 = 19.28

Days 19 – 23 = 19.93

Days 20 – 24 = 21.05

Days 21 – 25 = 22.05

Days 22 – 26 = 23.18

Days 23 – 27 = 24.13

Days 24 – 28 = 25.13

Days 25 – 29 = 26.00

Days 26 – 30 = 26.80

Days 27 – 31 = 27.45

Days 28 – 32 = 27.80

Days 29 – 33 = 27.90Sell signal (day 33 price < moving

average)

Days 30 – 34 = 28.20

Days 31 – 35 = 28.45

Days 32 – 36 = 28.65

Days 33 – 37 = 29.05

Days 34 – 38 = 29.25

Days 35 – 39 = 29.00

Days 36 – 40 = 28.75

19. This pattern shows a lack of breadth. Even though the index is up,

more stocks declined than advanced, which indicates a “lack of

broad-based support” for the rise in the index.

20.

Day Advances Declines

Net

Advances

Cumulative

Breadth

1 906 704 20

2 202

2 65

3 986 -333 -131

3 721 789 - 68 -199

4 503 968 -46

5 -664

5 497 1,095 -598 -1,262

6

970 702 268 -994 7

1,002 609 393 -601 8

903 722 181 -420 9

850 748 102 -318 10 766 766 0 -318

The signal is bearish as cumulative breadth is negative; however, the negative number is declining in magnitude, indicative of improvement. Perhaps the worst of the bear market has passed.

21. Trin =

936.0906/m illion 330704/m illion 240advancing Num ber /advancing V olum e declining Num ber /declining V olum e == This is a slightly bullish indicator, with average volume in advancing issues a bit greater than average volume in declining issues.

22. Confidence Index =

bonds corporate grade -te intermedia on Y ield bonds corporate rated -on top Y ield This year: Confidence index = (8%/10.5%) = 0.762

Last year: Confidence index = (8.5%/10%) = 0.850

Thus, the confidence index is decreasing.

23. Note: In order to create the 26-week moving average for the S&P 500,

we converted the weekly returns to weekly index values, with a base of 100 for the week prior to the first week of the data set. The

following graph shows the S&P 500 values and the 26-week moving

average, beginning with the 26th week of the data set.

a. The graph summarizes the data for the 26-week moving average.

The graph also shows the values of the S&P 500 index.

b. The S&P 500 crosses through its moving average from below 14

times, as indicated in the table below. The index increases

seven times in weeks following a cross-through and decreases

seven times.

Date of Cross-Through

Direction of S&P 500 in Subsequent

Week

05/18/01 Decrease 06/08/01 Decrease 12/07/01 Decrease 12/21/01 Increase 03/01/02 Increase 11/22/02 Increase 01/03/03 Increase

03/21/03 Decrease 04/17/03 Increase 06/10/04 Decrease 09/03/04 Increase 10/01/04 Decrease 10/29/04 Increase

04/08/05 Decrease

c. The S&P 500 crosses through its moving average from above 14

times, as indicated in the table below. The index increases nine times in weeks following a cross-through and decreases five times.

Date of Cross-Through Direction of S&P

500 in

Subsequent Week

Date of

Cross-

Through

Direction of S&P

500 in Subsequent

Week

06/01/01 Increase 03/28/03 Increase

06/15/01 Increase 04/30/04 Decrease

12/14/01 Increase 07/02/04 Decrease

02/08/02 Increase 09/24/04 Increase

04/05/02 Decrease 10/15/04 Decrease

12/13/02 Increase 03/24/05 Increase

01/24/03 Decrease 04/15/05 Increase

d. When the index crosses through its moving average from below,

as in part (b), this is regarded as a bullish signal. In our

sample, the index is as likely to increase as it is to

decrease following such a signal. When the index crosses

through its moving average from above, as in part (c), this is

regarded as a bearish signal. In our sample, contrary to the

bearish signal, the index is actually more likely to increase

than it is to decrease following such a signal.

24. In order to create the relative strength measure, we converted the

weekly returns for the Fidelity Banking Fund and for the S&P 500 to weekly index values, using a base of 100 for the week prior to the first week of the data set. The first graph shows the resulting

values, along with the relative strength measure (× 100). The

second graph shows the percentage change in the relative strength measure over five-week intervals.

a. The following graph summarizes the relative strength data for

the fund.

b. Over five-week intervals, relative strength increased by more

than 5% 29 times, as indicated in the table and graph below.

The Fidelity Banking Fund underperformed the S&P 500 index 18 times and outperformed the S&P 500 index 11 times in weeks

following an increase of more than 5%.

Date of Increase

Performance of

Banking Fund in

Subsequent Week

Date of

Increase

Performance of

Banking Fund in

Subsequent Week

07/21/00 Outperformed 03/09/01 Outperformed 08/04/00 Outperformed 03/16/01 Underperformed 08/11/00 Underperformed 03/30/01 Underperformed 08/18/00 Outperformed 06/22/01 Underperformed 09/22/00 Outperformed 08/17/01 Underperformed 09/29/00 Underperformed 03/15/02 Outperformed 10/06/00 Underperformed 03/22/02 Underperformed 12/01/00 Underperformed 03/28/02 Outperformed 12/22/00 Underperformed 04/05/02 Outperformed 12/29/00 Outperformed 04/12/02 Underperformed 01/05/01 Underperformed 04/26/02 Outperformed 01/12/01 Underperformed 05/03/02 Underperformed 02/16/01 Underperformed 05/10/02 Underperformed 02/23/01 Outperformed 06/28/02 Underperformed 03/02/01 Underperformed

c. Over five-week intervals, relative strength decreases by more

than 5% 15 times, as indicated in the graph above and table below. The Fidelity Banking Fund underperformed the S&P 500 index six times and outperformed the S&P 500 index nine times in weeks following a decrease of more than 5%.

Date of Decrease

Performance of

Banking Fund in

Subsequent Week

Date of

Decrease

Performance of

Banking Fund in

Subsequent Week

07/07/00 Underperformed 04/16/04 Underperformed

07/14/00 Outperformed 04/23/04 Outperformed

05/04/01 Underperformed 12/03/04 Outperformed

05/11/01 Outperformed 12/10/04 Underperformed

10/12/01 Outperformed 12/17/04 Outperformed

11/02/01 Outperformed 12/23/04 Underperformed

10/04/02 Outperformed 12/31/04 Underperformed

10/11/02 Outperformed

d. An increase in relative strength, as in part (b) above, is

regarded as a bullish signal. However, in our sample, the

Fidelity Banking Fund is more likely to underperform the S&P 500 index than it is to outperform the index following such a signal.

A decrease in relative strength, as in part (c), is regarded as

a bearish signal. In our sample, contrary to the bearish signal,

the Fidelity Banking Fund is actually more likely to outperform

the index increase than it is to underperform following such a

signal.

25. It has been shown that discrepancies of price from net asset value in

closed-end funds tend to be higher in funds that are more difficult to arbitrage such as less-diversified funds.

CFA PROBLEMS

1. i. Mental accounting is best illustrated by Statement #3.

Sampson’s requirement that his income needs be met via

interest income and stock dividends is an example of mental

accounting. Mental accounting holds that investors segregate

funds into mental accounts (e.g., dividends and capital gains),

maintain a set of separate mental accounts, and do not combine

outcomes; a loss in one account is treated separately from a

loss in another account. Mental accounting leads to an investor

preference for dividends over capital gains and to an inability

or failure to consider total return.

ii. Overconfidence(illusion of control) is best illustrated by Statement #6. Sampson’s desire to select investments that are

inconsistent with his overall strategy indicates

overconfidence. Overconfident individuals often exhibit risk-

seeking behavior. People are also more confident in the

validity of their conclusions than is justified by their

success rate. Causes of overconfidence include the illusion of

control, self-enhancement tendencies, insensitivity to

predictive accuracy, and misconceptions of chance processes.

iii. Reference dependence is best illustrated by Statement #5.

Sampson’s desire to retain poor-performing investments and to

take quick profits on successful investments suggests reference

dependence. Reference dependence holds that investment

decisions are critically dependent on the decision-maker’s

reference point. In this case, the reference point is the

original purchase price. Alternatives are evaluated not in

terms of final outcomes but rather in terms of gains and losses

relative to this reference point. Thus, preferences are

susceptible to manipulation simply by changing the reference

point.

2. a. Frost's statement is an example of reference dependence. His

inclination to sell the international investments once prices

return to the original cost depends not only on the terminal

wealth value, but also on where he is now, that is, his

reference point. This reference point, which is below the

original cost, has become a critical factor in Frost’s

decision.

In standard finance, alternatives are evaluated in terms of

terminal wealth values or final outcomes, not in terms of gains

and losses relative to some reference point such as original cost.

b. Frost’s statement is an example of susceptibility to cognitive

error, in at least two ways. First, he is displaying the

behavioral flaw of overconfidence. He likely is more confident

about the validity of his conclusion than is justified by his

rate of success. He is very confident that the past performance

of Country XYZ indicates future performance. Behavioral

investors could, and often do, conclude that a five-year record

is ample evidence to suggest future performance. Second, by

choosing to invest in the securities of only Country XYZ, Frost

is also exemplifying the behavioral finance phenomenon of asset

segregation. That is, he is evaluating Country XYZ investment

in terms of its anticipated gains or losses viewed in isolation.

Individuals are typically more confident about the validity of

their conclusions than is justified by their success rate or by

the principles of standard finance, especially with regard to

relevant time horizons. In standard finance, investors know

that five years of returns on Country XYZ securities relative

to all other markets provide little information about future

performance. A standard finance investor would not be fooled by

this “law of small numbers.” In standard finance, investors

evaluate performance in portfolio terms, in this case defined

by combining the Country XYZ holding with all other securities

held. Investments in Country XYZ, like all other potential

investments, should be evaluated in terms of the anticipated

contribution to the risk–reward profile of the entire

portfolio.

c. Frost’s statement is an example of mental accounting. Mental

accounting holds that investors segregate money into mental

accounts (e.g., safe versus speculative), maintain a set of

separate mental accounts, and do not combine outcomes; a loss

in one account is treated separately from a loss in another

account. One manifestation of mental accounting, in which Frost

is engaging, is building a portfolio as a pyramid of assets,

layer by layer, with the retirement account representing a

layer separate from the speculative fund. Each layer is

associated with different goals and attitudes toward risk. He

is more risk averse with respect to the retirement account than

he is with respect to the speculative fund account. The money

in the retirement account is a downside protection layer,

designed to avoid future poverty. The money in the speculative

fund account is the upside potential layer, designed for a

chance at being rich.

In standard finance, decisions consider the risk and return

profile of the entire portfolio rather than anticipated gains

or losses on any particular account, investment, or class of

investments. Alternatives should be considered in terms of

final outcomes in a total portfolio context rather than in

terms of contributions to a safe or a speculative account.

Standard finance investors seek to maximize the mean-variance

structure of the portfolio as a whole and consider covariances

between assets as they construct their portfolios. Standard

finance investors have consistent attitudes toward risk across

their entire portfolio.

3. a. Illusion of knowledge: Maclin believes he is an expert on, and

can make accurate forecasts about, the real estate market

solely because he has studied housing market data on the

Internet. He may have access to a large amount of real estate-

related information, but he may not understand how to analyze

the information nor have the ability to apply it to a proposed

investment.

Overconfidence: Overconfidence causes us to misinterpret the

accuracy of our information and our skill in analyzing it. Maclin

has assumed that the information he collected on the Internet is

accurate without attempting to verify it or consult other sources.

He also assumes he has skill in evaluating and analyzing the real

estate-related information he has collected, although there is no

information in the question that suggests he possesses such

ability.

b. Reference point: Maclin’s reference point for his bond

position is the purchase price, as evidenced by the fact that he

will not sell a position for less than he paid for it. This

fixation on a reference point, and the subsequent waiting for

the price of the security to move above that reference point

before selling the security, prevents Maclin from undertaking a

risk/return-based analysis of his portfolio position.

c. Familiarity: Maclin is evaluating his holding of company stock

based on his familiarity with the company rather than on sound

investment and portfolio principles. Company employees, because

of this familiarity, may have a distorted perception of their

own company, assuming a “good company” will also be a good

investment. Irrational investors believe an investment in a

company with which they are familiar will produce higher returns

and have less risk than nonfamiliar investments.

Representativeness: Maclin is confusing his company (which may

well be a good company) with the company’s stock (which may or

may not be an appropriate holding for his portfolio and/or a good

investment) and its future performance. This can result in

employees’ overweighting their company stock, thereby holding an

underdiversified portfolio.

4. a. The behavioral finance principle of biased

expectations/overconfidence is most consistent with the inve stor’s

first statement. Petrie stock provides a level of confidence and

comfort for the investor because of the circumstances in which she

acquired the stock and her recent history with the returns and

income from the stock. However, the investor exhibits

overconfidence in the stock given the needs of her portfolio (she

is retired) and the brevity of the recent performance history.

b. The behavioral finance principle of mental accounting is most

consistent with the investor’s second statement. The investor

has segregated the monies distributed from her portfolio into two “accounts”: the returns her portfolio receives on the Petrie stock and the returns on the rest of her portfolio. She is maintaining a separate set of mental accounts with regard to the total funds distributed. The investor’s specific uses should be viewed in the overall context of her spending needs and she should consider the risk and return profile of the entire portfolio.

5. i. Overconfidence (Biased Expectations and Illusion of Control):

Pierce is basing her investment strategy for supporting her

parents on her confidence in the economic forecasts. This is a

cognitive error reflecting overconfidence in the form of both

biased expectations and an illusion of control. Pierce is likely

more confident in the validity of those forecasts than is

justified by the accuracy of prior forecasts. Analysts’ consensus

forecasts have proven routinely and widely inaccurate. Pierce also

appears to be overly confident that the recent performance of the

Pogo Island economy is a good indicator of future performance.

Behavioral investors often conclude that a short track record is

ample evidence to suggest future performance.

Standard finance investors understand that individuals typically

have greater confidence in the validity of their conclusions than

is justified by their success rate. The calibration paradigm,

which compares confidence to predictive ability, suggests that

there is significantly lower probability of success than the

confidence levels reported by individuals. In addition, standard

finance investors know that recent performance provides little

information about future performance and are not deceived by this

“law of small numbers.”

ii. Loss Aversion (Risk Seeking): Pierce is exhibiting risk aversion in deciding to sell the Core Bond Fund despite its gains and

favorable prospects. She prefers a certain gain over a possibly

larger gain coupled with a smaller chance of a loss. Pierce is

exhibiting loss aversion (risk seeking) by holding the High Yield

Bond Fund despite its uncertain prospects. She prefers the modest

possibility of recovery coupled with the chance of a larger loss

over a certain loss. People tend to exhibit risk seeking, rather

than risk aversion, behavior when the probability of loss is large.

There is considerable evidence indicating that risk aversion holds

for gains and risk seeking behavior holds for losses, and that

attitudes toward risk vary depending on particular goals and

circumstances.

Standard finance investors are consistently risk averse and

systematically prefer a certain outcome over a gamble with the same

expected value. Such investors also take a symmetrical view of

gains and losses of the same magnitude, and their sensitivity

(aversion) to changes in value is not a function of a specified value reference point.

iii. Reference Dependence: Pierce’s inclination to sell her Small Company Fund once it returns to her original cost is an

example of reference dependence. This is predicated on the

current value as related to original cost, her reference point.

Her decision ignores any analysis of expected terminal value

or the impact of this sale on her total portfolio. This

reference point of original cost has become a critical but

inappropriate factor in Pierce’s dec ision.

In standard finance, alternatives are evaluated in terms of

terminal wealth values or final outcomes, not in terms of gains and losses relative to a reference point such as original cost.

Standard finance investors also consider the risk and return

profile of the entire portfolio rather than anticipated gains or losses on any particular investment or asset class.

相关主题